The role of attachment dimensions in adaptive and maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation with the mediation of cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts, social self-efficacy and self-disclosure

Number of pages: 232 File Format: word File Code: 29960
Year: 2014 University Degree: PhD Category: Educational Sciences
  • Part of the Content
  • Contents & Resources
  • Summary of The role of attachment dimensions in adaptive and maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation with the mediation of cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts, social self-efficacy and self-disclosure

    Doctoral dissertation in the field of educational psychology

    Abstract

    The role of attachment dimensions in adaptive and non-adaptive solutions

    Emotion regulation through the mediation of cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts,

    Social self-efficacy and self-disclosure

    The aim of the research The purpose of this study was to examine the role of attachment dimensions (including anxiety and attachment avoidance) in different emotional regulation strategies (including adaptive strategies, non-adaptive strategies based on overactivation of the attachment system and non-adaptive strategies based on deactivation of the attachment system) with the mediation of variables related to "self" (including social self-efficacy and self-disclosure) and the cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts. The research participants included 384 female and male undergraduate students of Yazd University who were selected by multi-stage cluster method.

    Questionnaires of attachment dimensions (Collins, 1996), emotion regulation strategies (Carver et al., 1989), social self-efficacy (Smith and Betz, 2000), self-disclosure (Kahn and Hessling, 2001) and cognitive suppression capacity. Unwanted thoughts (Washburn and Putney, 1998 computer software) were used and the results of factor analysis and Cronbach's alpha also confirmed their validity and reliability. The results of the present study based on a causal model with manifest and latent variables showed attachment anxiety, adaptive strategies to regulate emotions negatively (directly and mediated by social self-efficacy), strategies It predicts hyperactivation positively (directly and mediated by social self-efficacy) and deactivation strategies negatively (directly and mediated by lack of cognitive capacity). Also, attachment avoidance predicts adaptive strategies negatively (directly), hyperactivation strategies negatively (directly and mediated by self-disclosure), and deactivation strategies positively (directly and mediated by self-disclosure and lack of cognitive capacity). In addition, the proposed model of the research also had a favorable fit.

    In general, it can be acknowledged that the dimensions of attachment in adulthood predict the strategies used by a person in the face of stressors. Both anxiety and avoidance lead to the non-use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies, and each of them in some way leads a person to their own maladaptive strategies. Variables related to "self" and the cognitive capacity of people also mediate this relationship.

    Key words: attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, adaptive emotion regulation strategies, non-adaptive emotion regulation strategies, social self-efficacy, self-disclosure and cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts. 

    Introduction

    1-1- General

    Excitement is a main phenomenon in human functions and plays a significant role in the survival and adaptation of the human species (Kettlar[1], 2004; Gross[2] and Thompson[3], 2007; Ness[4] and Ellsworth[5], 2009). At the interpersonal level, emotions help a person to be aware of the inner states and behavioral intentions of others (Freija[6], 1986); The exchange of such information between people is necessary for human relations and is an important determinant in psychological and social well-being. In addition, emotion has intrapersonal functions, such as finding insight into personal values, which is an important motivational factor in decision-making (Bechara [7], Damasio [8] and Damasio, 2000). Of course, in addition to adaptive functions, emotions also have harmful effects, which are mainly caused by their appearance at the wrong time or intensity. Inappropriate emotional responses in many forms of psychopathology (Campbell-Sales[9] and Barlow[10], 2007; Linehan[11], Bohas[12] and Lynch[13], 2007), social problems (Vranik[14], Barrett[15] and Salovey[16], 2007; Isenberg[17], Hofer[18] and Vaughan[19], 2007; Shaver[20] and Michalinser[21], 2007) and even play a role in physical diseases. Therefore, successful emotional regulation plays a fundamental role in human life (Gross, 2007). Many psychological, cognitive, physiological and behavioral functions are dependent on people's success in emotion regulation (Gross and Thompson, 2007) and effective emotional regulation functions moderate the impact of people's evaluations and mental reactions in dealing with stressful events and lead to appropriate and needed cognitive, motivational and behavioral reactions in such situations (Kramer [22], 1991; Lazarus [23], 1991; Gross and Thompson, 2007).In the meantime, some researchers have emphasized only one aspect of the regulation, adaptation, or adjustment processes of emotion regulation, and some have focused on several aspects of the emotion regulation process at the same time (Campos [24] and Sternberg [25], 1981, cited by Gross and Thompson, 2007; Gross, 1998). For example, Lazarus (1991) has focused more on the cognitive dimension and sees emotion and cognitive systems as integrated, he believes that emotion and its regulation are the result of cognitive evaluation. On the other hand, Gross (1998) and Power [26] and Daglish [27] (2008) believe that emotional regulation includes simultaneous changes related to each other in the physiological system, behavior and cognitive processes. In this regard, it seems that Thompson (1991) provided the most comprehensive definition of emotion regulation. He defines emotion regulation as follows: "All the internal and external processes that are responsible for monitoring, evaluating and modifying emotional reactions (especially their intensity and characteristics) so that in this way it can achieve its goals" (p. 27).

    Emotion regulation is a group of innate characteristics that can be seen in infants, but it is significantly influenced by the environment and especially the parenting methods of parents. Emotion regulation develops in a nurturing environment, and otherwise, it functions maladaptively. The family, child-rearing methods and the areas that are influenced by them have been of interest to researchers in the family field to clarify its role in the development of emotion and emotional regulation. But the most comprehensive classification of emotion regulation strategies can be found in the model of Carver [28], Shir [29] and Weintraub [30] (1989), according to which fifteen types of emotion regulation are mentioned, including active adaptation [31], planning [32], suppression of other activities [33], inhibition [34], seeking instrumental social support [35], relying on emotional social support [36], reinterpretation It is positive [37], acceptance [38], denial [39], inclination to religion [40], lack of mental involvement [41], lack of behavioral involvement [42], focus on emotion [43], joking [44] and thirdly substance use [45].

    Recent research on family factors (including parent-child attachment) is rooted in Balbi's attachment theory [46] (1969). Balbi (1969, 1973, 1980, 1982) believes that the infant is born with a repertoire of behaviors (attachment behaviors) aimed at seeking closeness[47] with supportive others (attachment images[48]). In this view, proximity seeking is an innate emotion regulation device [49] (primary attachment solution [50]) that protects a person from facing physical and psychological threats and reduces distress. Balby (1988) believes that emotion regulation occurs best when a person has attachment security, i.e. achieves a sense that the world is a safe place. It is in this state that a person can rely on supportive others, confidently explore the environment and communicate with others.

    Among the popular models regarding emotional regulation is the Shaver and Mikalinser (2002) model, which emphasized the role of attachment in explaining the development of emotional regulation, the present research is also based on this model. Michalinser

    Shaver and Michalinser (2002) model has integrated the findings of Balby (1973, 1982), Ainsworth[51] (1991), Cassidy[52] and Kubak[53] (1988) and Farley[54] and Shaver (2000). In this model, there are three main parts. The first part includes the awareness and evaluation of threatening events, which is responsible for activating the primary attachment strategy, i.e. proximity seeking. In other words, if danger signs are received, proximity search is activated. The second part includes awareness and assessment of the availability [55] of external or internalized attachment patterns. Based on this, people act differently based on their attachment type, in the sense that if attachment images were available, the person would be directed to use safety-based strategies [56]. But if attachment diagrams are not available, the third part of the model appears, which examines and evaluates the possibility of proximity[57]. This part leads people to secondary attachment strategies, i.e. hyperactivation [58] or deactivation [59].

    Shiver and Michalinser (2002) in explaining these three parts, acknowledge that when a person faces a threat, his attachment system is activated.

  • Contents & References of The role of attachment dimensions in adaptive and maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation with the mediation of cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts, social self-efficacy and self-disclosure

    List:

    List of Contents

    Page Title

    Chapter One: Introduction

    1-1- Generality .. 2

    1-1- Model of the Short and Micalineser. 5

    1-1-2- Social self-efficacy and self-disclosure. 9

    1-1-3- cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts. 16

    1-2- Statement of the problem.. 19

    1-2-1- Research questions. 23

    1-3- Objectives of research.. 24

    1-4- Importance and necessity of research. 25

    Chapter Two: Theoretical foundations and previous researches

    2-1- Emotion and emotion regulation. 29

    2-1-1- Emotion regulation solutions. 42

    2-2- Attachment.. 50

    2-3- Shaver and Michalinser model. 61

    2-3-1- Activating the attachment system and primary attachment solution. 62

    2-3-2- Access to attachment diagrams and safety-based solutions. 62

    2-3-3- The possibility of searching for proximity and secondary attachment solutions. 64

    2-3-4- The growth of solutions based on attachment. 66

    2-3-4-1- Development of safety-based solutions. 66

    2-3-4-2- Development of secondary attachment strategies. 70

    2-4- attachment and emotion regulation. 72

    2-5- Social self-efficacy and self-disclosure. 82

    2-5-1- functional models of attachment. 83

    2-5-2- Social self-efficacy. 86

    2-5-3- self-disclosure. 104

    Title

    2-6- Cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts. 114

    2-7- Conclusion from previous research. 121

    2-7-1- Research hypotheses. 122

    Chapter three: research method

    3-1- Type of research.. 124

    3-2- Research participants. 124

    3-3- Research tools.. 125

    3-3-1- Scale of attachment dimensions. 125

    3-3-2- Emotion regulation solutions. 127

    3-3-3- Social self-efficacy. 131

    3-3-4- self-disclosure. 133

    3-3-5- Cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts. 134

    3-4- Implementation method.. 137

    3-5- Statistical analysis method. 138

    1-5-3- Model fit indices. 140

    Chapter Four: Research Findings

    4-1- Descriptive findings of obvious research variables. 148

    4-2- Preliminary analysis.. 150

    4-4- Evaluation of the measurement part of the model. 153

    4-5- Structural model evaluation. 158

    Chapter Five: Discussion and Conclusion

    5-1- Attachment anxiety and emotion regulation strategies. 172

    5-2- The mediating role of social self-efficacy and lack of cognitive capacity in the relationship between

    attachment anxiety and emotion regulation strategies. 176

    5-3- Attachment avoidance and emotion regulation solutions. 181

    Title

    4-5- Mediating role of self-disclosure and lack of cognitive capacity in the relationship between avoidance

    attachment and emotion regulation strategies. 184

    5-5- Themes and proposals of the research. 188

    6-5- Research limitations. 190

     

    Resources

    Persian sources.. 191

    English sources.. 192

     

    Appendices

    Appendix 1: Scale of attachment dimensions. 216

    Appendix 2: emotion regulation tool. 217

    Appendix 3: Self-disclosure scale. 219

    Appendix 4: Social self-efficacy tool. 220

    Appendix 5: Tools for creating negative emotions. 221 Source: Persian sources Khodapanahi, Mohammad Karim, and Heydari, Mahmoud. (1383). Examining the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs, self-restraint and academic success. Journal of Psychology, 8, 3, 227-218.

    Bashart, Mohammad Ali. (1388). Attachment styles and emotional failure. Psychological researches, volume 12, numbers 3 and 4, pp. 63-80.

    Hijazi Elaha; Farsinejad, Masoumeh and Asgari, Ali. (1386). Identity styles and academic achievement: the role of academic self-efficacy. Journal of Psychology, 11, 4, 413-394.

    Hekmatinejad, Iqbal. (1380). Investigating the interaction of gender and self-efficacy on the academic achievement of third year middle school students. Shiraz University, master thesis.

    Rahimi, Mehdi. (2011). Predicting the decision making process byPredicting the decision-making process by motivation, emotions and cognitive evaluations of emotions with the mediation of metacognitive tendencies: an integrated approach in decision-making. Shiraz University, unpublished doctoral thesis.

    Abdi, Salman; Babapour Khairuddin, Jalil, and Fathi, Haider. (1389). The relationship between cognitive emotion regulation styles and general health of students. Journal of the University of Medical Sciences of the Islamic Republic of Iran Army, 8, 4, 264-258.

    Foladcheng, Mehbobeh. (1383). Investigating the effect of teaching self-management skills and increasing self-sufficiency beliefs on the academic performance of high school students. Shiraz University, unpublished doctoral thesis.

    Homan, Heydar Ali. (2011). Modeling structural equations using Lisrel software (with modifications). Tehran Publishing: Organization for Studying and Compiling Humanities Books of Universities (Samt), Research and Development Center for Humanities.

    Yousfi, Farideh. (1385). Investigating the relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depression and anxiety in students of gifted guidance centers. Research in Exceptional Children, 6, 4, 871-892.

    English sources

    Adler, A. (1937). Position in family constellation influences life style. Journal of individual psychology, 3, 211-227.

    Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1991). Attachment and other affective bonds across the life cycle. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. ??Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 33–51). New York: Routledge.

    Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: Assessed in the Strange Situation and at home. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Aldwin, C., Revenson, T. A. (1987). Does coping help? A reexamination of the relationship between coping and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 337-348.

    Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). When are adaptive strategies most predictive of psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121, 276-281.

    Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Ang, R. P. (2006). Effects of parenting style on personal and social variables for Asian Adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(4), 503-511.

    Appleton, A.A., Buka, S.L., Loucks, E.B., Gilman, S.E., Kubzansky, L.D. (2013). Divergent associations of adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies with inflammation. Health Psychology, 32(7), 748-756.

    Archer, R. L., Berg, J. H., & Runge, T. E. (1980). Active and passive observer's attraction to a self-disclosing other. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 16, 130-145.

    Aron, A., & Aron, E. N. (2006). Romantic relationships from the perspectives of the self-expansion model and attachment theory. In M. Mikulincer & G. S. Goodman (Eds.), Dynamics of romantic love: Attachment, caregiving, and sex (pp. 359–382). New York: Guilford Press.

    Arbano, C., & Power, T. (2003). Parental attachment, self-esteem and antisocial behaviors among African American, European American, & Mexican American adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40-51.

    Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information.  Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461–484.

    Baldwin, S. A., & Hoffmann, J. P. (2002). The dynamics of self-esteem: A growth-curve anal-

    ysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,31, 101–113.

    Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning Press.

    Bandura, A. (1996). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. In G. H. Jennings & D. Belanger (Eds.), Passages beyond the gate: A Jungian approach to understanding the nature of American psychology at the dawn of the new millennium (pp. 107-96). Needham Heights, MA: Simon & Schuster.

     

    Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

The role of attachment dimensions in adaptive and maladaptive strategies of emotion regulation with the mediation of cognitive capacity to suppress unwanted thoughts, social self-efficacy and self-disclosure