The effectiveness of emotion regulation training on preschool children's social competence

Number of pages: 55 File Format: word File Code: 29242
Year: Not Specified University Degree: Not Specified Category: Educational Sciences
  • Part of the Content
  • Contents & Resources
  • Summary of The effectiveness of emotion regulation training on preschool children's social competence

    Dissertation for Master's Degree

    General Psychology Department

    Winter 91

     

    Abstract

     

     

    The present study was conducted to investigate the effectiveness of emotion regulation training on the social competence of preschool children. In this research, an experimental design with an experimental and control group was used. The statistical population includes all preschool children in Tehran. Using the multi-stage cluster sampling method, two kindergartens were selected, one of these kindergartens was selected as the experimental group and the second as the control group. Emotion regulation training included 11 lessons, both sessions were held in one week for 14 weeks (along with supplementary sessions). The social behavior evaluation test adapted from Weir and Davin (1980) was filled by educators in two stages before training and after training to evaluate the changes made in children's social behavior. And independent t statistic method was used for data analysis. The difference between the pre-test and post-test scores in the experimental group was larger and more significant than the control group. Emotion regulation training has been effective on preschool children's social competence. Key words: Emotion regulation training [1], social competence [2], preschool children [3]

    Chapter 1

    Research framework

    Introduction and problem statement

    One of the factors of interest of researchers in the social aspects of development during the past few decades, The relationship between problems in relationships with peers and poor academic, emotional and social adjustment has been investigated (Hartup[4], 1992; Coppersmith, Koye and Dagg[5], 1990; Newcomb[6] et al., 1993; Parker and Asher[7], 1987; Parker et al., 1995; quoted by Rose-Krasnor [8], 1997). These researchers agree that problems with peers in children can manifest themselves in the form of abnormal behaviors [9] (such as destructive behavior, aggression) or internalization [10] (such as withdrawal, social anxiety) (cited by Rose-Krasnor, 1997). This is why, in recent years, researchers, psychologists, educators and parents of children, in addition to emphasizing learning to read and math calculations in preschool, consider it necessary to achieve another important developmental task: achieving abilities to succeed in social relationships (Brophy-Hebb [11] et al., 2007, Brown [12], 2011).  The psychological structure of "social competence[13]" is a good representation of these abilities; Social competence in early childhood [14] means the child's participation in social interactions, achieving social goals, creating and maintaining friendships, and achieving peer acceptance (Robin, Bukowski, and Parker [15], 2006; quoted by Mirabel [16], 2010).

    Emotions are one of the main elements of social interactions, because they are an important source of information for both the person making the communication and the person receiving the communication. The emotional content of the relationship often determines the meaning of the relationship (Campus, Campos and Barrett [17], 1989; quoted by Halberstead, Denham and Dunsmore [18], 2011). The importance of emotions in social relationships is evident from the fact that some have said that it is difficult to imagine even a relationship without emotional information, to receive emotional information and to experience the emotional state (Halberstad, Denham, and Dunsmore, 2011). Managing emotions in a form that is compatible with the environment is necessary for social functioning and psychological well-being (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksma, Switzer [19], 2010; Denham, 1998; Isenberg). , Phibes, Gutierrez and Reiser [20], Gross [21], cited by McLaughlin, Menin and Nolen-Hoeksma [22]).  Thompson [23] (1994) has defined emotion regulation as "the internal and external processes that are responsible for awareness, evaluation and modification of the occurrence, duration and intensity of emotional reactions in order to achieve the individual's desires". "theory of mind" and "emotion understanding", the first of which means a child's developing understanding of the mental states of others, including their beliefs, thoughts, and desires (Dunn, Brown, Slomsky, Tesla, and Youngblade [24], 1991; cited by Lucas and Adkins, 2006) and the second is the child's ability to decode the emotional states of another person (Harris and Sarney [25]) , 1989; quoted by Lucas and Adkins, 2006). It has been found that the development of both of these skills helps to develop social competence (Barnett and Thompson, 1984, page 259; cited in Lucas-Adkins, 2006, Raza and Blair [26], 2009).

    It has been found that children's social competence may depend to some extent on the ability to accurately recognize and infer the emotional states of another person (Barnett and Thompson [27], 1984, page 259; quoted by Lucas-Adkins [28], 2006). River and Wiggles [29] (1997; cited by Brophy-Hebb et al., 2007) note that social competence depends on the acquisition of basic skills such as emotion regulation [30], social cognition skills [31] and positive communication behaviors. Children and adolescents with better emotion regulation skills have more social competence, have a higher rank among peers, and their relationships are of higher quality, and in comparison With peers with poor emotion regulation skills, they are in a higher category of socially desirable behavior [32] (Denham, 1986; Denham et al., 2003; Denham, McKinley, Kuchod, and Holt [33], 1990; quoted by McLogin, Hesenbuhler, Menin, and Nolen-Hoeksma, 2011). It has been found that emotion regulation skills can predict children's social competence in later stages of development (Denham et al., 2003; Riedel [34] et al., 2003; Spinard [35] et al., 2006; cited by McLaughin, Hesenbuehler, Menin, and Nolen-Hoeksma, 2011). Collectively, they are called emotional competence [36] (Halberstatt, Denham and Dunsmore, 2001; Sarni [37], 2000).  Children in preschool age gain emotional awareness. As a result, they are better able to talk about their feelings and respond appropriately to other people's emotional signals. They become better at emotional self-regulation [38], especially in coping with intense negative emotion. Preschool children often experience self-aware emotions [39] and empathy [40] that help their developing moral understanding. They point to the causes and consequences and behavioral symptoms of excitement, and these awarenesses increase rapidly in terms of accuracy and complexity. They can also predict what a teammate will do next when they express a certain emotion. They acquire effective ways to soothe others' negative emotions, such as hugging to relieve sadness. At the same time, preschool children have difficulty interpreting situations with conflicting signs about how a person feels. When 4- and 5-year-old children were shown a picture of a happy-looking child with a broken bicycle, they relied on an expression of excitement and said, "He is happy because he likes to ride his bicycle." Older children usually combine these two signs: "He is happy because his father promised to fix his bicycle" (Burke [41], 2007). Also, because preschool children are not aware of mental activities, they explain emotions by referring to external states instead of internal ones (Flavel and Green [42], 2001). Children are almost 8 years old when they become aware that they can experience more than one emotion at any moment, so that each one may be positive or negative and differ in intensity (Ponce [43] et al., 2003). Understanding mixed emotions [44] helps children to understand that the expression of people's emotions may not reflect their real feelings (Sarni, 1999) and also helps to be aware of conscious emotions.  Based on the research findings mentioned above, children's emotional capabilities increase with age and the development of emotion has a developmental process, which is a testimony of the mutual relationship between cognition and emotion that the researcher will describe in detail in the next part of the research. The core of Jean Piaget's theoretical position [45] was that children should build their mental and physical structures through activity and action in the environment (Piaget, 1982; quoted by Feldman and Fuller [46], 1998). When children directly influence the environment, these structures change and their thinking develops. Despite Piaget's many but unsatisfactory efforts to explain this theoretical position, he received many criticisms; Other theorists believed that growth and learning are mutually related, and contrary to Piaget's opinion, they did not consider the relationship between growth and learning to be one-way, where only growth leads to learning. One of the strict supporters of the position of learning, who announced his position many years ago, was Lev Vygotsky[47]. According to him, learning is a process through which the possibility of increasing conceptual structures is created.

  • Contents & References of The effectiveness of emotion regulation training on preschool children's social competence

    Persian sources

    Burke, Laura (2007). Developmental psychology (from conception to childhood), 4th edition. Translated by Yahya Seyed Mohammadi. Tehran: Arsbaran Publishing House, 1389

    Jadidi, Houshang (1375). Examining the effect of parental behavior patterns on the ability to solve social problems of Khomin city students. Unpublished master's thesis of the University of Tehran.

    Crane, William C. (1998). Theories of Rushd, translated by Alireza Khoinejad and Gholamreza Safaei. Rushd Publications, 1384. English sources: Adela Moraru, Mihaela Stoica, Elena-Adriana Tomuletiu, Filpisan Monica. (2011). Evaluation of a program for developing socio-emotional competence in preschool children. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences.(30), pp(2161-2164).

    Berk, L.E. (2007).Child development. Allyn and Bacon. Boston, U.S.A

    Bierman KL, Domittrovich CE, Nix RL, Gest SD, Welsh JA, Greenberg MT, Blair C, Nelson KE, Gills.(2008). Promoting academic and social-emotional school readiness: the headstart REDI program. Child development(79), pp(17-1802).

    Bradley Raymond Trevor, Atkinson Mike, Tomasio. Dana, Rees Robert A. (2009). Facilitating Emotional Self-regulation in preschool children: efficacy of the early.

    Brophy-Herb Holly E. Robert E. Lee, M. Angela Nievar, Gary Stollak. (2007). Preschooler 'ssocial competence: Relations to family characteristics, teacher behaviors and classroom climate. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. (28), pp (134-148).

    Brown, A. Chavaughn. (2011). The association between social-emotional competence and children's academic readiness, Doctoral dissertation. George Mason university.

    Cole, M., John-Steiner.V., Scribner, S., and Souberman, E. (Eds). (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard university press. Ole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scr Mind in society: the development ~[" hi

                         

    Elias.J Maurice, Gara.A Michael, Schuyler.F Thomas, Branden.R Leslie, Sayette.A Michael.(2010). The promotion of social competence: Longitudinal study of a preventive school-based program. American Journal of orthopsychiatry. Volume 61, Issue 3, pages 409-417

    Feldman, David henri. Flower, R. Clarke (1997). The natures of developmental change: Piaget, Vygotsky, and the transition process. New Ideas in psychol. 3, pp (195-210). Bobbitt, Edstrom Leihua Van Schoiack, Hirschstein Miriam K. (2005). Effects of a school-based social-emotional competence program: Linking children's goals, attributions and behavior. Applied developmental psychology (26), pp(171-200).

    Greenberg Mark.T, Kusche Carol.A.(2010).A introduction to promoting alternative thinking strategies.The public of department of health and human services.www.samhsa.gov

    Halberstadt Amy. G, Denham Susanne. A, Dunsmore Julie. C. (2001). Affective social competence. Department of psychology. North Carolina State University.

    Hennessey Betha.A(2007).Promotion social competence in school-aged children: The effects of the open circle program.Journal of school psychology.(45),pp(349-360).

    Macklem, Gayle L.(2007).Practitioner's Guide to Emotion Regulation in School-aged Children.

    McLaughin Katie, A., Hazenbuehler. Mark L, Mennin Douglas, S ., Nolen-Hoeksema Susan.( 2011). Emotion dysregulatio and adolescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behavioral Research and Therapy.(49), pp (544-554).

    Raver, C. C., & Zigler, E. F. (1997). Social competence: An untapped dimension in evaluating Head Start's success. Early Childhood Research

    Quarterly, 12, 363?385.

    Razza, Rachel A., Blair, Clancy. (2009). Associations among false-belief understanding, executive function, and social competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of applied developmental psychology. ( 30 ), pp ( 332-343 ).

    Rose-Krasnor, Linda(1997).The Nature of Social Competence: A Theoretical Review, Psychology department.Brock University.Correspondence can also be addressed to [email protected]

    Saarni, C.(1999).The development of emotional competence, NY: Guilford Press.

    Thompson, R.A. (1994). Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monograph for the society for research in child development. (59), pp (25-52).

    Saarni, C(.2000). The development of emotional competence, NY: Guilford Press.

     

    Spinard, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., et al. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social competence: a longitudinal study. Emotion, 6, 498e510.

    Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development.

    Educational Psychology Review, 3, 269–307.

    Thompson, R.A. (1994). Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monograph for the society for research in child development. (59), pp. (25-52).

The effectiveness of emotion regulation training on preschool children's social competence